Logo-hpp
2023: Two-year Impact Factor: 2.4
Scopus Journal Metrics
CiteScore (2023):7.1
 
Platinum
Open Access

Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory

Health Promotion Perspectives, 8(4), 255-262; DOI:10.15171/hpp.2018.36

Review

Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory

Paul D. Loprinzi1 ,*

1 Exercise & Memory Laboratory, Department of Health, Exercise Science and Recreation Management, The University of Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA

Email: pdloprin@olemiss.edu

© 2018 The Author(s). This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background: The purpose of this review was to evaluate whether acute exercise intensities have unique effects on memory function, and whether this is influenced by memory type as well as the temporality of the acute exercise bout.Methods: A systematic review was employed, using several databases (PubMed, PsychInfo,Sports Discus, Google Scholar, Embase).Results: In total, 9 articles met the study criteria. All 9 studies evaluated either working memory capacity or episodic-related memory function. The main findings across these studies were 1) when acute exercise occurs before the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory but may favor episodic memory; 2) when acute exercise occurs during the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory capacity; and 3) high-intensity exercise may not associate with long-term memory function when it occurs shortly after memory encoding.Conclusion: The relationship between acute exercise and memory is complex and may vary based on the intensity of exercise, the temporality of exercise, and the memory type evaluated.

Keywords: Episodic memory, High-intensity, Moderate-intensity, Walking, Working memory

Citation: Loprinzi PD. Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory. Health Promot Perspect. 2018;8(4):255-262. doi: 10.15171/hpp.2018.36.

Introduction

Unquestionably, memory function is critical for optimal daily function. Retrospective memory recall refers to the ability to recall past events, with episodic memory defined as the retrospective recall of past episodes or events in a spatio-temporal context (what-where-when aspects of memory). In addition to episodic memory, another commonly evaluated memory type includes working memory capacity, which refers to the transient repository of information, often while occurring during the concurrent processing of other information (e.g., remembering your hotel room number during the day).

Emerging work suggests that acute exercise (and chronic) may subserve both episodic and working memory capacity.1-5 We have previously discussed the potential mechanisms through which exercise may influence episodic memory function.6,7 These postulated exercise-related mechanisms include, for example, 1) Enhanced neuronal excitability; 2) Enhanced attentional resource allocation to facilitate memory encoding; 3) Upregulation of AMPA receptor levels, opening NMDA channels, and increasing EPSP (excitatory post-synaptic potentials) in the hippocampus; 4) The priming of neurons to be encoded in the memory trace by increasing CREB transcription; 5) BDNF (brain-derived neurotropic factor) expression; and 6) Enhanced dendritic spine growth. Notably, these exercise-induced changes are likely to occur in brain structures (e.g., prefrontal cortex and hippocampus) that subserve memory function. In the context of working memory, acute exercise may enhance this memory type via modulation of norepinephrine and dopamine levels.8 For example, D1 receptor stimulation enhances the excitability of prefrontal pyramidal cells and potentiates glutamate gated currents.8 Importantly, working memory is optimized at intermediate levels of D1 receptor stimulation and is degraded by either too little, or too much activation of this dopamine receptor. Thus, exercise intensity may play a critical role in memory function.

The purpose of this brief review was to evaluate the literature to examine the extent to which acute exercise intensity influences memory function. Although various reviews have discussed the effects of exercise on memory,3,5,6 no review, to my knowledge, has specifically evaluated intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory. Some, however, have looked to see if memory outcomes varied based on whether studies compared no exercise to vigorous-intensity exercise, or no exercise to moderate-intensity exercise.3 What is lacking in the literature is isolating studies that have made direct comparisons to multiple exercise intensities (i.e., control vs. moderate-intensity vs. high-intensity), which is the focus of the present review. Herein, I also examine whether memory type (e.g., working memory vs. episodic memory) and the temporal effects of exercise (i.e., acute exercise occurring before, during or after the memory task) play an important role in the exercise intensity-memory relationship.

Materials and Methods

Studies were identified using electronic databases, including PubMed, PsychInfo, Sports Discus, Google Scholar, and Embase.9 In alignment with PRISMA guidelines, computerized searches were conducted up until July 10, 2018, identifying articles published prior to this date (no restriction was placed on how far back the study was published). The search terms included: exercise; intensity; cognition; cognitive function; memory; low-intensity; moderate-intensity; and high-intensity (and their combinations).

To be eligible for inclusion in this systematic review, studies had to:

Be published in English.

Be conducted in humans.

Employ an experimental design.

Independent variable had to be a measure of acute exercise (not chronic exercise10-12). At least 2 different exercise intensity levels (not exercise duration13,14) had to be compared (e.g., moderate-intensity vs. high-intensity exercise).

Include a cognitive-related memory assessment (e.g., episodic memory, working memory). Studies that employed an overall cognitive function assessment that included memory as part of this global score were not included. Additionally, studies that focused on motor memory were excluded.15-18

Not include a stress-induced neurotoxicity paradigm (e.g., looking at the protective effects of exercise intensity on attenuating stress-induced memory impairment)19-22 or another type of environmental stimuli (e.g., hypoxia) that could alter memory function.23

Results

The computerized searches revealed 526 unique articles. The title and abstract of each of these articles were reviewed. Among these 526 articles, 9 articles met the study criteria listed above. Table 1 displays the extraction table for the results of the 9 evaluated studies.

Table 1. Extraction table of the evaluated studies
Study Subjects Study Design Exercise Protocol Memory Task Time Period of Assessments Findings
Winter et al24 30 healthy young males (~22 y) Experimental, between-subject design Control (15 min), 40-min of low impact running, and 2 sprints of 3-min at increasing speed Language learning paradigm (episodic memory) Exercise and then the memory assessment occurred immediately after exercise, 1-week later, and 6-8 months later. Immediate learning was slightly greater in the high-intensity group. No significant differences for the 1-week follow-up period, but the high-intensity group also had slightly greater memory retention. In the high-intensity exercise group, those with greater increases in epinephrine (vs. below median epinephrine levels) had greater memory retention at both the 1-week and 6-8 month follow-up periods.
Lo Bue-Estes et al25 18 youth women (18-25 y) Experimental, within-subject design Maximal bout of treadmill running Arithmetic calculations (working memory) Working memory assessment occurred pre-exercise, post-exercise, and then at 4 time-points during exercise (25%, 50%, 75%, and 100% of max). The assessments during occurred during a break between transitioning to the faster speed. Working memory declined during the exercise period but then was enhanced above baseline at the 30-min post-exercise period.
Budde et al26 60 healthy high school students (15-16 y) Experimental, between-subject design Control, 50-65% HRmax, and 70-85% HRmax. Exercise protocol included 12-min of running on a 400-m track Letter Digit Span Task (working memory) Memory assessment, exercise, 5-min rest, then memory assessment No statistically significant group x time interaction. However, the lower exercise-intensity group appeared to have greater increases in working memory. Low baseline performers on the working memory task also demonstrated suggestive evidence of improvements from high-intensity exercise.
Loprinzi and Kane27 87 young adults (~21 y) Experimental, between-subject design Control, 40%-50% HRmax, 51-70% HRmax, or 71%-85% HRmax. Each bout of exercise lasted 30-min. Spatial span; paired associates (short-term visuospatial memory) Exercise and then rested for 15-min (or heart rate was within 10% of resting) before starting the memory task No statistically significant group x time interaction.
Etnier et al2 16 young adults (~23 y) Experimental, within-subject design VO2max, Vt + 20%, Vt – 20%. The submaximal treadmill exercise bouts lasted 30 min. RAVLT (episodic memory) Exercise and then immediately commence memory task There was no significant group x time interaction effect for learning (RAVLT trials 1-5). Long-term memory (24-h follow-up) was greatest for the maximal exercise condition (VO2max).
Hotting et al28 81 young healthy adults (18-29 y) Experimental, between-subject design Control, <57% HRmax, ~80% HRmax. Sessions lasted 30-min. Cycle ergometer exercise. 20-item word list (episodic memory) Memory encoding, exercise (~10-min after memory encoding), then memory retrieval Those in the control group recalled more words at the two follow-up assessment periods (20-min and 24-h recall). The high-intensity group forgot fewer words across the two time points, but this could have been a result of a greater room for change in the control group.
Rattray and Smee29 20 healthy young adults (~26 y) Experimental, within-subject design Control, 90% Vt, 90% of Vt with pick-ups, and 90% of Vt with drop-downs. Exercise (cycle) bout lasted approximately 60-min. Speed Match task (similar to 1-back condition of n-back task). Measures working memory. Memory baseline, then exercise, and during the 50th and 55th min during exercise, memory was re-assessed. No significant condition by time interaction for memory accuracy.
Keyan and Bryant30 62 healthy young adults (~21 y) Experimental, between-subject design Walking slowly vs. stepping exercise (stepping up and down while hold 1 kg hand weight). Each exercise bout lasted 10-min. Exposed to images from the IAPS. Memory recall was assessed 2-days later. Memory recognition. Exercise and then immediately viewed the images Exercise participants recalled more emotional images than control participants, and more negative than positive images were recalled.
Tempest et al31 14 young adults (~23 y) Experimental, within-subject design High-intensity exercise (Vt + 10%) and very low-intensity (<30 W). Exercise bouts lasted 60-min and included cycle ergometer exercise 2-back task (working memory). Exercise, and while exercising, they completed the memory task. Over the bout of exercise, working memory declined in the high-intensity exercise session, with no change in the low-intensity session.

Among the 9 evaluated studies,24-31, all, with the exception of one (adolescents),26 were conducted among young adults (18-30 years). All 9 studies evaluated either working memory capacity or episodic-related memory function. Five studies employed a between-subject experimental design, with the other 4 employing a within-subject experimental design. The exercise protocols varied considerably, including a short maximal treadmill bout25; 40-minute of low impact running vs. 2 x 3 minute sprints24; 12-minutes of running at 50%-65% vs. 70%-85% HRmax26; 30-minutes of treadmill exercise at either 40%-50%, 51%-70%, or 71%-85% HRmax27; 30-minutes of treadmill exercise at Vt (ventilatory threshold) + 20%, Vt – 20%, or VO2max protocol2; 30-minutes of cycle exercise at <57 or 80% of HRmax28; 60-minutes of cycle exercise at 90% of Vt vs. 90% of Vt with intermittent sprints29; 10-minutes of walking slowly vs. stepping exercise with holding 1kg weight30; and 60-minutes of cycle exercise at Vt + 10% vs. <30 W.31

In addition to variations in the study design (between-subject vs. within-subject), memory type (working memory vs. episodic memory), and exercise protocol (cycling vs. treadmill; and considerable variations in different intensities), the temporal sequence of the exercise and memory assessments also varied. For example, some studies implemented the exercise protocol prior to the memory assessment,2,24,26,27,30 others evaluated memory during exercise,25,29,31 and others implemented the exercise bout after memory encoding (i.e., during early memory consolidation).28

Regarding the main findings, for the studies implementing the exercise protocol prior to the memory assessment,2,24,26,27,30 4 evaluated episodic memory and one evaluated working memory. In the working memory study,26 the initial main analysis did not demonstrate a statistically significant effect, but follow-up analyses demonstrated that the lower exercise-intensity group (50%-65% HRmax vs. 70%-85% HRmax) appeared to have greater increases in working memory, and low baseline performers on the working memory task also demonstrated suggestive evidence of improvements from high-intensity exercise. Among the 4 episodic memory studies, one study did not observe any intensity-specific differences on memory,27 whereas the other 3 studies provided evidence that the higher-intensity protocol was advantageous in memory.2,24,30 For example, one study24 showed that a few short sprints (3-minute) was more effective in enhancing learning when compared to a 40-minute low impact jog, and those with greater exercise-induced increases in epinephrine had greater memory retention at both the 1-week and 6-8 month follow-up periods. Another study2 demonstrated that a short-duration, maximal bout of exercise (vs. Vt + 20% and Vt-20%) was the most effective in enhancing long-term (24-hour follow-up) memory. Lastly, one study30 showed that 10-minute of stepping exercise with a hand-held weight (vs. slow walking) was more effective in enhancing memory recognition 2-days later. Taken together, among these 5 studies evaluating exercise before the memory task, these findings provide suggestive evidence that lower intensity exercise may be more beneficial for working memory, but higher-intensity exercise may favor episodic memory.

Three studies evaluated memory function during the exercise bout.25,29,31 All 3 of these studies evaluated working memory capacity. Over a 60-minute bout of cycle exercise, one study29 did not observe any intensity-dependent changes in working memory. However, the other 2 studies25,31 demonstrated that working memory function was impaired during a bout of high-intensity exercise when compared to lower-intensity exercise; i.e., during a maximal bout of exercise (vs. before and after)25 or cycle exercise at Vt + 10% vs. <30 watts.31 Taken together, among these 3 studies evaluating memory function during exercise (or during intermittent breaks25), these findings provide suggestive evidence that working memory capacity may be impaired during exercise, particularly for higher-intensity exercise.

Lastly, one study implemented the exercise bout after memory encoding (i.e., during early memory consolidation).28 This study evaluated episodic memory. This study showed that those in the control group recalled more words at the 2 follow-up assessment periods (20-minute and 24-hour recall). However, the high-intensity group forgot fewer words across the 2 time points, but this could have been a result of a greater capacity for change in the control group.

Discussion

The purpose of the present review was to evaluate whether there is an intensity-specific effect of acute exercise on memory function. The main findings of this review are as follows. Few studies have evaluated varying acute exercise intensities on memory function, and thus, it is not possible to provide strong conclusions. Despite this, these findings suggest that: 1) when acute exercise occurs before the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory but may favor episodic memory; 2) when acute exercise occurs during the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory capacity; and 3) high-intensity exercise may not associate with long-term memory function when it occurs shortly after memory encoding. The narrative that follows will discuss each of these 3 points.

The first observation of this review was that there was some suggestive evidence that when acute exercise occurs before the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory but may favor episodic memory. Regarding the timing of exercise, other work (not comparing different intensity levels) demonstrates that acute moderate- and high-intensity exercise before memory encoding is optimal when compared to other temporal periods.32-34, Differential effects of exercise intensity on working memory when compared to episodic memory, have been discussed elsewhere.35 Moderate-intensity exercise may favor working memory capacity, in particular. Both light and moderate-intensity acute exercise, but not high-intensity exercise, are associated with increased P3 amplitude in tasks related to information processing and executive function, whereas only moderate-intensity acute exercise is associated with shortened P3 in tasks involving executive function.36-38

High-intensity exercise, on the other hand, may increase levels of norepinephrine (NE) and dopamine (DA) in the prefrontal cortex, activating β-adrenoceptors and D1-receptors, respectively, ultimately activating cAMP, which may dampen neuronal activity (via cAMP opening of nearby K+ channels, which may weaken the effectiveness of nearby synaptic inputs39) in the prefrontal cortex, and potentially impair prefrontal cortex function.40 There appears to be an inverted U-shaped relationship between NE/DA and working memory function. High levels of NE activate α1 and β1 receptors, which in turn, facilitate glucocorticoid detrimental actions. Both NE and DA have complimentary roles, with high levels increasing the noise-to-signal ratio; at modest levels, D1 receptor activation increases neuronal firing by decreasing firing to non-preferred inputs (i.e., decreasing noise), whereas NE increases firing to preferred inputs (i.e., increasing signals).40 On the other hand, and in contrast to impaired working memory capacity in the prefrontal cortex, elevated levels of catecholamines can enhance synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus.41,42 For example, elevated levels of NE can induce various intracellular signaling pathways (e.g., PKA) to facilitate CREB transcription, and in turn, subserve long-term potentiation.43 Relatedly, animal work demonstrates that increased running speed is accompanied by systematic increases in the frequency of CA1 network oscillations spanning the gamma frequency range.44

Another observation from the evaluated studies of this review was that when acute exercise occurs during the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory capacity. This may be explained, in part, from the transient hypofrontality hypothesis,45,46 which hypotheses that during exercise there is a relative shift in neural activity away from the prefrontal cortex in order to sustain neural activation in other brain regions that are more critical for movement. Research shows that during exercise, performance on tasks demanding prefrontal-dependent cognition are impaired, while cognitive processes requiring little prefrontal activity are unaffected.47 I am not suggesting, however, that episodic memory function may be completely preserved during exercise, as the prefrontal cortex may still play an important role in episodic memory.48 Notably, and when compared to exercising prior to memory encoding, experimental work suggests that episodic memory may be slightly worse when memory encoding occurs during exercise.32,33,49

The remaining observation from this review was that high-intensity exercise may not associate with long-term memory function when it occurs shortly after memory encoding. Of course, this should be interpreted with caution as only a single study in this review evaluated a potential differential effect of post-exercise intensity on memory function.28 This finding aligns with other related research. Although not directly comparing different exercise intensity levels in a single study, other research suggest that exercising shortly after memory encoding is slightly less advantageous (when compared to exercising prior to encoding) for both moderate-intensity49-51, and high-intensity exercise.33 In addition to these laboratory findings evaluating exercise very shortly after memory encoding, other work in free-living settings shows that exercising 1-2 hours after memory encoding may also be less advantageous for long-term memory.52 Notably, however, emerging work suggests that exercising 4-hours after memory encoding is associated with enhanced long-term memory function.53 Future work is needed that examines different temporal periods of exercise post-encoding to evaluate whether there is an optimal time period to which exercise may enhance the consolidation of memories. Additionally, future research would benefit by evaluating the effects of anaerobic sprints on memory performance, as the studies evaluated herein did not impose short-duration (e.g., 60-seconds or less), high-intensity sprints.

In conclusion, this review suggests an intensity-specific effect of exercise on memory and also highlights that results may differ based on memory type and the temporality of memory assessment. Specifically, when acute exercise occurs before the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory but may favor episodic memory; when acute exercise occurs during the memory task, high-intensity exercise may be less favorable for working memory capacity; and high-intensity exercise may not associate with long-term memory function when it occurs shortly after memory encoding. Additional work exploring these interrelationships is needed.

Ethical approval

Not applicable.

Competing interests

None.

Funding

None.

Author’s contribution

PDL conceived the study and wrote the entire manuscript.

References

  1. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance:a meta-analysis. Brain Res 2012;1453:87-101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068. [Crossref]
  2. Etnier JL, Wideman L, Labban JD, Piepmeier AT, Pendleton DM, Dvorak KK, et al. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Memory and Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF). J Sport Exerc Psychol 2016;38(4):331-40. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0335. [Crossref]
  3. Roig M, Nordbrandt S, Geertsen SS, Nielsen JB. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory:a review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013;37(8):1645-66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012. [Crossref]
  4. Roig M, Thomas R, Mang CS, Snow NJ, Ostadan F, Boyd LA, et al. Time-dependent effects of cardiovascular exercise on memory. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 2016;44(2):81-8. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000078. [Crossref]
  5. Loprinzi PD, Frith E, Edwards MK, Sng E, Ashpole N. The effects of exercise on memory function among young to middle-aged adults: systematic review and recommendations for future research. Am J Health Promot. Am J Health Promot 2018;32(3):691-704. doi: 10.1177/0890117117737409. [Crossref]
  6. Loprinzi PD, Edwards MK, Frith E. Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways:a narrative review. Eur J Neurosci 2017;46(5):2067-77. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13644. [Crossref]
  7. Loprinzi PD, Frith E. A brief primer on the mediational role of BDNF in the exercise-memory link. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging 2018; In press. doi: 10.1111/cpf.12522. [Crossref]
  8. Khan ZU, Muly EC. Molecular mechanisms of working memory. Behav Brain Res 2011;219(2):329-41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.039. [Crossref]
  9. Bramer WM, Rethlefsen ML, Kleijnen J, Franco OH. Optimal database combinations for literature searches in systematic reviews:a prospective exploratory study. Syst Rev 2017;6(1):245. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0644-y. [Crossref]
  10. Jeon YK, Ha CH. The effect of exercise intensity on brain derived neurotrophic factor and memory in adolescents. Environ Health Prev Med 2017;22(1):27. doi: 10.1186/s12199-017-0643-6. [Crossref]
  11. Connolly LJ, Bailey SJ, Krustrup P, Fulford J, Smietanka C, Jones AM. Effects of self-paced interval and continuous training on health markers in women. Eur J Appl Physiol 2017;117(11):2281-93. doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3715-9. [Crossref]
  12. Moreau D, Kirk IJ, Waldie KE. High-intensity training enhances executive function in children in a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Elife 2017;6. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25062. [Crossref]
  13. Stevens DJ, Arciuli J, Anderson DI. Statistical learning is not affected by a prior bout of physical exercise. Cogn Sci 2016;40(4):1007-18. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12256. [Crossref]
  14. Crush EA, Loprinzi PD. Dose-response effects of exercise duration and recovery on cognitive functioning. Percept Mot Skills 2017;124(6):1164-93. doi: 10.1177/0031512517726920. [Crossref]
  15. Skriver K, Roig M, Lundbye-Jensen J, Pingel J, Helge JW, Kiens B, et al. Acute exercise improves motor memory:exploring potential biomarkers. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2014;116:46-58. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.08.004. [Crossref]
  16. Thomas R, Flindtgaard M, Skriver K, Geertsen SS, Christiansen L, Korsgaard Johnsen L, et al. Acute exercise and motor memory consolidation:Does exercise type play a role? Scand J Med Sci Sports 2017;27(11):1523-32. doi: 10.1111/sms.12791. [Crossref]
  17. Thomas R, Johnsen LK, Geertsen SS, Christiansen L, Ritz C, Roig M, et al. Acute exercise and motor memory consolidation: the role of exercise intensity. PLoS One 2016;11(7):e0159589. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159589. [Crossref]
  18. Statton MA, Encarnacion M, Celnik P, Bastian AJ. A single bout of moderate aerobic exercise improves motor skill acquisition. PLoS One 2015;10(10):e0141393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141393. [Crossref]
  19. Klein CP, Hoppe JB, Saccomori AB, Dos Santos BG, Sagini JP, Crestani MS, et al. Physical exercise during pregnancy prevents cognitive impairment induced by amyloid-beta in adult offspring rats. Mol Neurobiol 2018; In press. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1210-x. [Crossref]
  20. Wu Z, Li X, Zhang Y, Tong D, Wang L, Zhao P. Effects of sevoflurane exposure during mid-pregnancy on learning and memory in offspring rats: beneficial effects of maternal exercise. Front Cell Neurosci 2018;12:122. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00122. [Crossref]
  21. Cheng M, Cong J, Wu Y, Xie J, Wang S, Zhao Y, et al. Chronic swimming exercise ameliorates low-soybean-oil diet-induced spatial memory impairment by enhancing BDNF-mediated synaptic potentiation in developing spontaneously hypertensive rats. Neurochem Res 2018;43(5):1047-57. doi: 10.1007/s11064-018-2515-x. [Crossref]
  22. Miller RM, Marriott D, Trotter J, Hammond T, Lyman D, Call T, et al. Running exercise mitigates the negative consequences of chronic stress on dorsal hippocampal long-term potentiation in male mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2018;149:28-38. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.01.008. [Crossref]
  23. Seo Y, Gerhart HD, Stavres J, Fennell C, Draper S, Glickman EL. Normobaric hypoxia and submaximal exercise effects on running memory and mood state in women. Aerosp Med Hum Perform 2017;88(7):627-32. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4798.2017. [Crossref]
  24. Winter B, Breitenstein C, Mooren FC, Voelker K, Fobker M, Lechtermann A, et al. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2007;87(4):597-609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003. [Crossref]
  25. Lo Bue-Estes C, Willer B, Burton H, Leddy JJ, Wilding GE, Horvath PJ. Short-term exercise to exhaustion and its effects on cognitive function in young women. Percept Mot Skills 2008;107(3):933-45. doi: 10.2466/pms.107.3.933-945. [Crossref]
  26. Budde H, Voelcker-Rehage C, Pietrassyk-Kendziorra S, Machado S, Ribeiro P, Arafat AM. Steroid hormones in the saliva of adolescents after different exercise intensities and their influence on working memory in a school setting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010;35(3):382-91. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.07.015. [Crossref]
  27. Loprinzi PD, Kane CJ. Exercise and cognitive function:a randomized controlled trial examining acute exercise and free-living physical activity and sedentary effects. Mayo Clin Proc 2015;90(4):450-60. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.12.023. [Crossref]
  28. Hotting K, Schickert N, Kaiser J, Roder B, Schmidt-Kassow M. The effects of acute physical exercise on memory, peripheral BDNF, and cortisol in young adults. Neural Plast 2016;2016:6860573. doi: 10.1155/2016/6860573. [Crossref]
  29. Rattray B, Smee DJ. The effect of high and low exercise intensity periods on a simple memory recognition test. J Sport Health Sci 2016;5:342-8.
  30. Keyan D, Bryant RA. Role of BDNF val66met polymorphism in modulating exercised-induced emotional memories. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017;77:150-7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.013. [Crossref]
  31. Tempest GD, Davranche K, Brisswalter J, Perrey S, Radel R. The differential effects of prolonged exercise upon executive function and cerebral oxygenation. Brain Cogn 2017;113:133-41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.02.001. [Crossref]
  32. Sng E, Frith E, Loprinzi PD. Temporal effects of acute walking exercise on learning and memory function. Am J Health Promot 2018;32(7):1518-1525. doi: 10.1177/0890117117749476. [Crossref]
  33. Frith E, Sng E, Loprinzi PD. Randomized controlled trial evaluating the temporal effects of high-intensity exercise on learning, short-term and long-term memory, and prospective memory. Eur J Neurosci 2017;46(10):2557-64. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13719. [Crossref]
  34. Labban JD, Etnier JL. Effects of acute exercise on long-term memory. Res Q Exerc Sport 2011;82(4):712-21. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599808. [Crossref]
  35. McMorris T. Developing the catecholamines hypothesis for the acute exercise-cognition interaction in humans:Lessons from animal studies. Physiol Behav 2016;165:291-9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.011. [Crossref]
  36. Kamijo K, Nishihira Y, Hatta A, Kaneda T, Wasaka T, Kida T, et al. Differential influences of exercise intensity on information processing in the central nervous system. Eur J Appl Physiol 2004;92(3):305-11. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1097-2. [Crossref]
  37. Kamijo K, Nishihira Y, Higashiura T, Kuroiwa K. The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;65(2):114-21. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.001. [Crossref]
  38. Chang YK. Acute exercise and event-related potential: Current status and future prospects. In: McMorris T, ed. Exercise-Cognitive Interaction: Neuroscience Perspectives. New York: Academic Press; 2016. p. 105-30.
  39. Arnsten AF, Wang MJ, Paspalas CD. Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron 2012;76(1):223-39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038. [Crossref]
  40. Arnsten AF. Catecholamine influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical networks. Biol Psychiatry 2011;69(12):e89-99. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.027. [Crossref]
  41. Gagnon SA, Wagner AD. Acute stress and episodic memory retrieval:neurobiological mechanisms and behavioral consequences. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2016;1369(1):55-75. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12996. [Crossref]
  42. Hansen N. The longevity of hippocampus-dependent memory is orchestrated by the locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system. Neural Plasticity 2017;2017: 2727602. doi: 10.1155/2017/2727602. [Crossref]
  43. Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Memory modulation. Behav Neurosci 2011;125(6):797-824. doi: 10.1037/a0026187. [Crossref]
  44. Ahmed OJ, Mehta MR. Running speed alters the frequency of hippocampal gamma oscillations. J Neurosci 2012;32(21):7373-83. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5110-11.2012. [Crossref]
  45. Dietrich A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Res 2006;145(1):79-83. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.033. [Crossref]
  46. Dietrich A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: the transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Conscious Cogn 2003;12(2):231-56.
  47. Dietrich A, Sparling PB. Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain Cogn 2004;55(3):516-24. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.03.002. [Crossref]
  48. Preston AR, Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol 2013;23(17):R764-73. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041. [Crossref]
  49. Haynes Iv JT, Frith E, Sng E, Loprinzi PD. Experimental Effects of Acute Exercise on Episodic Memory Function: Considerations for the Timing of Exercise. Psychol Rep 2018;33294118786688. doi: 10.1177/0033294118786688.
  50. Siddiqui A, Loprinzi PD. Experimental investigation of the time course effects of acute exercise on false episodic memory. J Clin Med 2018;7(7). doi: 10.3390/jcm7070157.
  51. Salas CR, Minakata K, Kelemen WL. Walking before study enhances free recall but not judgement-of-learning magnitude. J Cogn Psychol (Hove) 2011;23(4):507-13. doi:10.1080/20445911.2011.532207. [Crossref]
  52. Pontifex MB, Gwizdala KL, Parks AC, Pfeiffer KA, Fenn KM. The association between physical activity during the day and long-term memory stability. Sci Rep 2016;6:38148. doi: 10.1038/srep38148. [Crossref]
  53. van Dongen EV, Kersten IH, Wagner IC, Morris RG, Fernandez G. Physical exercise performed four hours after learning improves memory retention and increases hippocampal pattern similarity during retrieval. Curr Biol 2016;26(13): 1722-7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.071. [Crossref]
Submitted: 27 Aug 2018
Revised: 17 Sep 2018
Accepted: 22 Sep 2018
First published online: 27 Oct 2018
EndNote EndNote

(Enw Format - Win & Mac)

BibTeX BibTeX

(Bib Format - Win & Mac)

Bookends Bookends

(Ris Format - Mac only)

EasyBib EasyBib

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Medlars Medlars

(Txt Format - Win & Mac)

Mendeley Web Mendeley Web
Mendeley Mendeley

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Papers Papers

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

ProCite ProCite

(Ris Format - Win & Mac)

Reference Manager Reference Manager

(Ris Format - Win only)

Refworks Refworks

(Refworks Format - Win & Mac)

Zotero Zotero

(Ris Format - FireFox Plugin)

Abstract View: 2905
PDF Download: 1661
Full Text View: 1146
CC-BY © 2018 The Author(s). This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided theoriginal work is properly cited.